The Reading Room
Mind Games: Psychological Warfare Between Therapists and Scientists
By CAROL TAVRIS
Recently, while lecturing to a large group of lawyers, judges,
mediators, and others involved in the family-court system in Los
Angeles, I asked how many knew what a "social psychologist"
was. Three people shyly raised their hands. That response was typical,
and it´s the reason I don´t tell people anymore that I´m a social
psychologist: They think I´m a therapist who gives lots of parties. If
I tell them I´m a psychological scientist, they think I´m a pompous
therapist, because everyone knows that "psychological science"
is an oxymoron.
In fact, in many states, I cannot call myself a psychologist at all
-- the word is reserved for someone who has an advanced degree in
clinical psychology and a license to practice psychotherapy. That
immediately rules out the many other kinds of psychologists who conduct
scientific research in their respective specialties, including child
development, gerontology, neurobiology, emotions, sleep, behavioral
genetics, memory and cognition, sexual behavior and attitudes, trauma,
learning, language, . . . and social psychology, the study of how social
situations and other people affect every human activity from love to
war.
For the public, however, the word "psychologist" has only
one meaning: psychotherapist. It is true that clinical psychologists
practice therapy, but many psychologists are not clinicians, and most
therapists are not clinical psychologists. The word
"psychotherapist" is completely unregulated. It includes
people who have advanced training in psychology, along with those who
get a "certification" in some therapeutic specialty; clinical
social workers; marriage, family, and child counselors; psychoanalysts
and psychiatrists; and countless others who have no training in
anything. Starting tomorrow, I could package and market my own highly
effective approach, Chocolate Immersion Therapy, and offer a weekend
workshop to train neophytes ($395, chocolate included). I could carry
out any kind of unvalidated, cockamamie therapy I wanted, and I would
not be guilty of a single crime. Unless I described myself as a
psychologist.
As a result of such proliferation of psychotherapists, the work of
psychological scientists who do research and teach at colleges and
universities tends to be invisible outside the academy. It is the
psychotherapists who get public attention, because they turn up on talk
shows, offer advice in books and newspaper columns, and are interviewed
in the aftermath of every disaster or horrible crime -- for example,
speculating on the motives and childhoods of the Washington snipers. Our
society runs on the advice of mental-health professionals, who are often
called upon in legal settings to determine whether a child has been
molested, a prisoner up for parole is still dangerous, a defendant is
lying or insane, a mother is fit to have custody of her children, and on
and on. Yet while the public assumes, vaguely, that therapists must be
"scientists" of some sort, many of the widely accepted claims
promulgated by therapists are based on subjective clinical opinions and
have been resoundingly disproved by empirical research conducted by
psychological scientists. Here are a few examples that have been shown
to be false:
Low self-esteem causes aggressiveness, drug use, prejudice, and low
achievement.
Abused children almost inevitably become abusive parents, causing a
"cycle of abuse."
Therapy is beneficial for most survivors of disasters, especially if
intervention is rapid.
Memory works like a tape recorder, clicking on at the moment of
birth; memories can be accurately retrieved through hypnosis, dream
analysis, or other therapeutic methods.
Traumatic experiences, particularly of a sexual nature, are typically
"repressed" from memory, or split off from consciousness
through "dissociation."
The way that parents treat a child in the first five years (three
years) (one year) (five minutes) of life is crucial to the child´s
later intellectual and emotional success.
Indeed, the split between the research and practice wings of
psychology has grown so wide that many psychologists now speak glumly of
the "scientist-practitioner gap," although that is like saying
there is an "Arab-Israeli gap" in the Middle East. It is a
war, involving deeply held beliefs, political passions, views of human
nature and the nature of knowledge, and -- as all wars ultimately do --
money and livelihoods. The war spilled out of academic labs and
therapists´ offices and into the public arena in the 1980s and ´90s,
when three epidemics of hysteria caught fire across the country: the
rise of claims of "repressed memories" of childhood sexual
abuse; the growing number of cases of "multiple-personality
disorder" (MPD), from a handful before 1980 to tens of thousands by
1995; and the proliferation of day-care sex-abuse scandals, which put
hundreds of nursery-school teachers in prison on the
"testimony" of 3and 4-year-old children.
All three epidemics were fomented and perpetuated by the mistaken
beliefs of psychotherapists: that "children never lie about sexual
abuse"; that childhood trauma causes the personality to
"split" into several or even thousands of identities; that if
you don´t remember being sexually abused in childhood, that´s evidence
that you were; that it is possible to be raped by your father every day
for 16 years and to "repress" the memory until it is
"uncovered" in therapy; that hypnosis, dream analysis, and
free association of fantasies are reliable methods of
"uncovering" accurate memories. (On the contrary, such
techniques have been shown to increase confabulation, imagination, and
memory errors, while inflating the belief that the retrieved memories
are accurate.) The epidemics began to subside as a result of the
painstaking research of psychological scientists.
But psychotherapeutic nonsense is a Hydra: Slay one set of mistaken
ideas, and others take their place. Recovered-memory therapy may be on
the wane, but "rebirthing" techniques and forms of
"restraint therapy" -- physically abusive practices that
supposedly help adopted or troubled children form attachments to their
parents -- are on the rise. In Colorado, 10-year-old Candace Newmaker
was smothered to death during rebirthing, a procedure in which she was
expected to fight her way through a "birth canal" of
suffocating blankets and pillows. The two therapists convicted in
Candace´s death are now serving time in prison, but efforts in Colorado
to prohibit all forms of "restraint therapy" were defeated by
protests from "attachment therapists" in the state and
throughout the country. After Candace´s death, one member of the
Colorado Mental Health Grievance Board noted with dismay that her
hairdresser´s training took 1,500 hours, whereas anyone could take a
two-week course and become "certified" in rebirthing. Yet the
basic premise -- that children can recover from trauma, insecure
attachment, or other psychological problems by "reliving"
their births or being subjected to punitive and coercive restraints --
has no scientific validity whatsoever.
To understand how the gap between psychological scientists and
clinicians grew, it is necessary to understand a little about therapy
and a little about science, and how their goals and methods diverged.
For many years, the training of most clinical psychologists was based on
a "scientist practitioner" model. Ideally, clinicians would
study the research on human behavior and apply relevant findings to
their clinical practice. Clinical psychologists who are educated at
major universities are still trained in this model. They study, for
example, the origins of various mental disorders and the most effective
ways to treat them, such as cognitive-behavior therapy for anxiety,
depression, eating disorders, anger, and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
They have also identified which interventions are unhelpful or
potentially harmful. For example, independent assessments of a popular
post-trauma intervention called Critical Incident Stress Debriefing have
found that most survivors benefit just as much by talking with friends
and other survivors as with debriefers. Sometimes CISD even slows
recovery, by preventing victims from drawing on their own wellsprings of
resilience. And, sometimes, it harms people -- for example, by having
survivors ventilate their emotions without also learning good methods of
coping with them.
Unfortunately, the numbers of scientifically trained clinicians have
been shrinking. More and more therapists are getting their degrees from
"free-standing" schools, so called because they are
independent of research institutions or academic psychology departments.
In these schools, students are trained only to do therapy, and they do
not necessarily even learn which kinds of therapy have been shown to be
most effective for particular problems. Many of the schools are
accredited by the American Psychological Association, and their
graduates learn what they need to know to pass state licensing
examinations. But that does not mean that the graduates are
scientifically knowledgeable. For example, the Rorschach Inkblot Test
has been resoundingly discredited as a reliable means of diagnosing most
mental disorders or emotional problems; it usually reveals more about
the clinician administering it than about the individual taking it. I
call it the Dracula of psychological tests, because no one has been able
to drive a stake through the cursed thing´s heart. Many clinicians love
it; it is still widely used; and it still turns up on licensing exams.
Of course, tensions exist between researchers and practitioners in
any field -- medicine, engineering, education. Whenever one group is
doing research and the other is working in an applied domain, their
interests and training will differ. The goal of the clinician, in
psychology or medicine, is to help the suffering individual; the goal of
the psychological or medical researcher is to explain and predict the
behavior or course of illness in people in general. That is why many
clinicians argue that empirical research cannot possibly capture the
complex human beings who come to their offices. Professional training,
they believe, should teach students empathy and appropriate therapeutic
skills. Good therapy depends on the therapist´s insight and experience,
not on knowledge of statistics, the importance of control groups, and
the scientific method.
I agree that therapy often deals with issues on which science is
silent: finding courage under adversity, accepting loss, making moral
choices. My clinician friends constantly impress me with their deep
understanding of the human condition, which is based on seeing the human
condition sobbing in their offices many times a week. Nor am I arguing
that psychological scientists, or any other kind, are white knights with
a special claim to intellectual virtue. They, too, wrangle over data,
dispute each other furiously in print and public, and have plenty of
vested interests and biases. (For example, many scientists and consumer
advocates are concerned about the growing co-optation of scientific
investigators by the pharmaceutical industry -- which now finances the
majority of studies of treatments for mental disorders and sexual
problems -- because the result has been a pro-drug bias in research.)
It is not that I believe that science gives us ultimate truths about
human behavior, while clinical insight is always foolish and wrong.
Rather, I worry that when psychotherapists fail to keep up with basic
research on matters on which they are advising their clients; when they
fail to learn which methods are most appropriate for which disorders,
and which might be harmful; when they fail to understand their own
biases of perception and do not learn how to correct them; when they
fail to test their own ideas empirically before running off to promote
new therapies or wild claims -- then their clients and the larger public
pay the price of their ignorance.
For present purposes, I am going to do an end run around the
centuries-old debate about defining science, and focus on two core
elements of the scientific method. These elements are central to the
training of all scientists, but they are almost entirely lacking in the
training of most psychotherapists, including clinical psychologists. The
first is skepticism: a willingness to question received wisdom. The
second is a reliance on gathering empirical evidence to determine
whether a prediction or belief is valid. You don´t get to sit in your
chair and decide that autism is caused by cold, rejecting,
"refrigerator" mothers, as Bruno Bettelheim did. But legions
of clinicians (and mothers) accepted his cruel and unsubstantiated
theory because he was, well, Bruno Bettelheim. It took skeptical
scientists to compare the mothers of autistic children with those of
healthy children, and to find that autism is not caused by anything
parents do; it is a neurological disorder.
The scientific method is designed to help investigators overcome the
most entrenched human cognitive habit: the confirmation bias, the
tendency to notice and remember evidence that confirms our beliefs or
decisions, and to ignore, dismiss, or forget evidence that is
discrepant. That´s why we are all inclined to stick to a hypothesis we
believe in. Science is one way of forcing us, kicking and screaming if
necessary, to modify our views. Most scientists regard a central, if not
defining, characteristic of the scientific method to be what Karl Popper
called "the principle of falsifiability": For a theory to be
scientific, it must be falsifiable -- you can´t show me just those
observations that confirm it, but also those that might show it to be
wrong, false. If you can twist any result of your research into a
confirmation of your hypothesis, you aren´t thinking scientifically.
For that reason, many of Freud´s notions were unfalsifiable. If
analysts saw evidence of "castration anxiety" in their male
patients, that confirmed Freud´s theory of its universality; if
analysts didn´t see it, Freud wrote, they lacked observational skills
and were just too blind or stubborn to see it. With that way of
thinking, there is no way to disconfirm the belief in castration
anxiety.
Yet many psychotherapists perpetuate ideas based only on confirming
cases -- the people they see in therapy -- and do not consider the
disconfirming cases. The popular belief in "the cycle of
abuse" rests on cases of abusive parents who turn up in jail or
therapy and who report that they were themselves victims of abuse as
children. But scientists would want to know also about the disconfirming
cases: children who were beaten but did not grow up to mistreat their
children (and, therefore, did not end up in therapy or jail), and people
who were not beaten and then did grow up to be abusive parents. When the
researchers Joan Kaufman and Edward Zigler reviewed longitudinal studies
of the outcomes of child abuse, they found that although being abused
does considerably increase the risk of becoming an abusive parent, more
than 70 percent of all abused children do not mistreat their offspring
-- hardly an inevitable "cycle."
Practitioners who do not learn about the confirmation bias and ways
to counteract it can make devastating judgments in court cases. For
example, if they are convinced that a child has been sexually molested,
they are often unpersuaded by the child´s repeated denials; such
denials, they say, are evidence of the depth of the trauma. Sometimes,
of course, that is true. But what if it isn´t? In the Little Rascals
day-care-abuse case in North Carolina, one mother told reporters that it
took 10 months before her child was able to "reveal" the
molestation. No one at the time considered the idea that the child might
have been remarkably courageous to persist in telling the truth for so
long.
Because many therapists tend not to be as deeply imbued with the
spirit of skepticism as scientists are (or are supposed to be), it is
common for many of them to place their faith in the leader of a
particular approach, and to set about trying to do what the school´s
founder did -- rather than to raise too many questions about the founder´s
methods or the validity of the founder´s theories. If you go off to
become certified in Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR),
invented by Francine Shapiro while she was walking in the woods one day,
you are unlikely to ask, "Why, exactly, does waving your finger in
front of someone´s eyes realign the halves of the brain and reduce
anxiety?" Scientific studies of this method show that the
successful ingredient in EMDR is an old, tried-and-true technique from
behavior therapy: exposing people to a thought or situation that makes
them anxious, until the feeling subsides. The eye movements that are
supposedly essential, the clinical scientist Scott O. Lilienfeld
concluded, do not constitute "anything more than pseudoscientific
window dressing."
Similarly, most clinicians are not trained to be skeptical of what a
client says or to demand corroborating evidence. Why would they be? A
client comes to see you complaining that he has a terrible mother; are
you going to argue? Ask to meet the mother? Some clinicians, notably
those who practice cognitive-behavior therapy, would, indeed, ask you
for the evidence that your mother is terrible and also invite you to
consider other explanations of her behavior; but most do not. As the
psychiatrist Judith Herman explained in a PBS Frontline special on
recovered memory: "As a therapist, your job is not to be a
detective; your job is not to be a fact-finder; your job is not to be a
judge or a jury; and your job is also not to make the family feel
better. Your job is to help the patient make sense out of her life, make
sense out of her symptoms . . . and make meaning out of her
experience."
That remark perfectly summarizes the differing goals of most
clinicians and scientists. Clinicians are certainly correct that most of
the time it is not possible to corroborate a client´s memory anyway,
and that it isn´t their job to find out what "really"
happened in the client´s past. Scientists, though, have shown that
memories are subject to distortion. So, if the client is going to end up
suing a parent for sexual abuse, or if the therapist´s intervention
ends up causing a devastating family rift, a little detective work seems
called for. Detective work is the province of scientists, who are
trained not to automatically believe what someone says or what someone
claims to remember, but to ask, "Where´s the evidence?"
For psychological scientists, clinical insight is simply not
sufficient evidence. For one thing, the clinician´s observations of
clients will be inherently limited if they overlook comparison groups of
people who are not in therapy. For example, many clinicians invent
"checklists" of "indicators" of some problem or
disorder -- say, that "excessive" masturbation or bed-wetting
are signs of sexual abuse or, my favorite, that losing track of time or
becoming engrossed in a book is a sign of multiple-personality disorder.
But, before you can say that bed-wetting or masturbation is an indicator
that a child has been sexually abused, what must you know? Many
psychotherapists cannot give you the simple answer: You must know the
rates of bed-wetting and masturbation among all children, including
nonabused ones. In fact, many abused children have no symptoms, and many
nonabused children wet their beds, masturbate, and are fearful in new
situations.
Throughout the 1980s and ´90s, many therapists routinely testified
in court that they could magically tell, with complete certainly, that a
child had been sexually abused because of how the child played with
anatomically correct dolls, or because of what the child revealed in
drawings. The plausible assumption is that very young children may
reveal feelings in their play or drawings that they cannot express
verbally. But while such tests may have a therapeutic use, again the
scientific evidence is overwhelming that they are worthless for
assessment or diagnostic purposes. How do we know that? Because when
scientists compared the doll play of abused children to that of control
groups of nonabused children, they found that such play is not a valid
way of determining whether a child has been sexually abused. The doll´s
genitals are pretty interesting to all kids.
Likewise, psychological scientists who study children´s cognitive
development empirically have examined the belief held by many
psychotherapists that "children never lie" about sexual abuse.
Scientists have shown in dozens of experiments that children often do
tell the truth, but that they also lie, misremember, and can be
influenced to make false allegations -- just as adults do. Researchers
have shown, too, that adults often misunderstand and misinterpret what
children say, and they have identified the conditions that increase a
child´s suggestibility and the interviewing methods virtually
guaranteed to elicit false reports. Those conditions and methods were
present in the interrogations of children by social workers, therapists,
and police officers in all of the sensational cases of day-care hysteria
of the 1980s and ´90s. And those coercive practices continue in many
jurisdictions today where child-protection workers have not been trained
in the latest research.
I fear that the scientist-therapist gap is a done deal. There are too
many economic and institutional supports for it, in spite of yearly
exhortations by every president of the American Psychological
Association for "unity" and "cooperation." That´s
why, in the late 1980s, a group of psychological scientists formed their
own organization, the American Psychological Society, to represent their
own scientific interests. Every year, the APA does something else to
rile its scientific members while placating its therapist members --
like supporting prescription-writing privileges for Ph.D. psychologists
and approving continuing-education programs for unvalidated methods or
tests -- and so, every year, more psychological scientists leave the APA
for the APS.
But to the public, all this remains an internecine battle that seems
to have no direct relevance. That´s the danger. Much has been written
about America´s scientific illiteracy, but social-scientific illiteracy
is just as widespread and in some ways even more pernicious. People can
deny evolution or fail to learn basic physics, but such ignorance rarely
affects their personal lives. The scientific illiteracy of
psychotherapists has torn up families, sent innocent defendants to
prison, cost people their jobs and custody of their children, and
promoted worthless, even harmful, therapies. A public unable to
critically assess psychotherapists´ claims and methods for scientific
credibility will be vulnerable to whatever hysterical epidemic comes
along next. And in our psychologically oriented culture, there will be
many nexts. Some will be benign; some will merely cost money; and some
will cost lives.
Carol Tavris, a social psychologist, is on the board of the Council
for Scientific Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry, a consulting editor
of The Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice, and a member of the
editorial board of Psychological Science in the Public Interest.
http://chronicle.com
Section: The Chronicle Review
Volume 49, Issue 25, Page B7
|